Skip to contents

Time models in calmr

Version 0.5 of calmr introduced its first time-based model, ANCCR (Jeong et al., 2022), and with it, I wrote several additional tools for future time-based models.

Changes to trial-based models

The biggest change in calmr version 0.5 is the use of the “>” character and its effect on trial-based models, or rather, the lack thereof.

Before, the “>” character was used to specify a single split within a trial. For example, “A>(US)” would encode the typical situation in which stimulus A is followed by the US. This was used to mimic the traditional situation in which we expect an organism to start (conditionally) responding before the US is delivered. And so, all trial-based models had two steps within each trial: an expectation step in which the first half of the trial retrieved (and responded to) absent stimuli, and a learning step, in which all stimuli in the trial were associated with each other. The first pass is no more (so start throwing extinction trials, or better yet, probe trials with the “#” character to test your associations).

Specifying a design for time-based models

The designs for time-based models are nearly identical to those for trial-based models. However, clever use of the “>” character will enrich them. Let’s specify a serial feature discrimination experiment:

library(calmr)
#> 
#> Attaching package: 'calmr'
#> The following object is masked from 'package:base':
#> 
#>     parse
fpfn <- data.frame(
  group = c("FP", "FN"),
  phase1 = c("100F>T>(US)/100T", "100F>T/100T>(US)"),
  r1 = c(TRUE, TRUE)
)
parse_design(fpfn)
#> CalmrDesign built from data.frame:
#>   group           phase1   r1
#> 1    FP 100F>T>(US)/100T TRUE
#> 2    FN 100F>T/100T>(US) TRUE
#> ----------------
#> Trials detected:
#>   group  phase trial_names trial_repeats is_test stimuli
#> 1    FP phase1    F>T>(US)           100   FALSE  F;T;US
#> 2    FP phase1           T           100   FALSE       T
#> 3    FN phase1         F>T           100   FALSE     F;T
#> 4    FN phase1      T>(US)           100   FALSE    T;US

We can manually specify the timing for the above experiment by calling the get_timings() function. Manipulating the list returned by that function will result in a manipulation of the timing between the experimental events.

ts <- get_timings(fpfn, model = "ANCCR")
ts
#> $use_exponential
#> [1] TRUE
#> 
#> $trial_ts
#>      trial post_trial_delay mean_ITI max_ITI
#> 1 F>T>(US)                1       30      90
#> 2        T                1       30      90
#> 3      F>T                1       30      90
#> 4   T>(US)                1       30      90
#> 
#> $transition_ts
#>      trial transition transition_delay
#> 1 F>T>(US)        F>T                1
#> 2 F>T>(US)     T>(US)                1
#> 3      F>T        F>T                1
#> 4   T>(US)     T>(US)                1

And now let’s get the parameters for the ANCCR model.

pars <- get_parameters(fpfn, model = "ANCCR")
# increase learning rates
pars$alpha_reward <- 0.8
pars$alpha <- 0.08
# increase sampling interval to speed up the model
pars$sampling_interval <- 5
pars
#> $reward_magnitude
#>  F  T US 
#>  1  1  1 
#> 
#> $betas
#>  F  T US 
#>  1  1  1 
#> 
#> $cost
#> [1] 0
#> 
#> $temperature
#> [1] 1
#> 
#> $threshold
#> [1] 0.6
#> 
#> $k
#> [1] 1
#> 
#> $w
#> [1] 0.5
#> 
#> $minimum_rate
#> [1] 0.001
#> 
#> $sampling_interval
#> [1] 5
#> 
#> $use_exact_mean
#> [1] 0
#> 
#> $t_ratio
#> [1] 1.2
#> 
#> $t_constant
#> [1] NA
#> 
#> $alpha
#> [1] 0.08
#> 
#> $alpha_reward
#> [1] 0.8
#> 
#> $use_timed_alpha
#> [1] 0
#> 
#> $alpha_exponent
#> [1] 1
#> 
#> $alpha_init
#> [1] 1
#> 
#> $alpha_min
#> [1] 0
#> 
#> $add_beta
#> [1] 0
#> 
#> $jitter
#> [1] 1

Let’s make the model’s experience and look at the first 20 entries.

experiment <- make_experiment(fpfn,
  parameters = pars,
  timings = ts,
  model = "ANCCR"
)
head(experiences(experiment)[[1]], 20)
#>    model group  phase tp       tn is_test block_size trial stimulus       time reward_mag
#> 1  ANCCR    FP phase1  2        T   FALSE          2     1        T   5.222588          1
#> 2  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     2        F  27.634165          1
#> 3  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     2        T  28.634165          1
#> 4  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     2       US  29.634165          1
#> 5  ANCCR    FP phase1  2        T   FALSE          2     3        T  50.407887          1
#> 6  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     4        F  73.612261          1
#> 7  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     4        T  74.612261          1
#> 8  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     4       US  75.612261          1
#> 9  ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     5        F 123.492008          1
#> 10 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     5        T 124.492008          1
#> 11 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     5       US 125.492008          1
#> 12 ANCCR    FP phase1  2        T   FALSE          2     6        T 178.746581          1
#> 13 ANCCR    FP phase1  2        T   FALSE          2     7        T 215.804318          1
#> 14 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     8        F 224.910748          1
#> 15 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     8        T 225.910748          1
#> 16 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2     8       US 226.910748          1
#> 17 ANCCR    FP phase1  2        T   FALSE          2     9        T 317.910748          1
#> 18 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2    10        F 321.908467          1
#> 19 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2    10        T 322.908467          1
#> 20 ANCCR    FP phase1  1 F>T>(US)   FALSE          2    10       US 323.908467          1

As you can see above, there are several rows per trial, each specifying a different stimulus. Time-based models like ANCCR run over a time log because they make ample use of the temporal difference between events.

Let’s run the model and see some plots.

experiment <- run_experiment(experiment)
# Action values
patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "action_values"))

# ANCCR
patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "anccrs"))

# Dopamine transients
patch_plots(plot(experiment, type = "dopamines"))

And that’s it! Easy, right?

References

Jeong, H., Taylor, A., Floeder, J. R., Lohmann, M., Mihalas, S., Wu, B., Zhou, M., Burke, D. A., & Namboodiri, V. M. K. (2022). Mesolimbic dopamine release conveys causal associations. Science (New York, N.Y.), 378, eabq6740. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abq6740